Mantids do not feel pain

Mantidforum

Help Support Mantidforum:

This site may earn a commission from merchant affiliate links, including eBay, Amazon, and others.
mantis feel pain.. they might not have as many nerves going through their body but they do have pain. like a small back leg will not have many if any nerves.

 
Arthropods do not have any kind of pain sensors. If you take out part of an insects leg, it does not limp but carries on walking with the leg it has left. There won't be signs of complaint.

What about parasitized insects? Like aphids or caterpillars that got into the way of a parasite wasp. Their larvae are eating the victims from the inside out and they don't seem to bother and continue their normal lives. Or even the unlucky mantids that get caught by a species of parasite fly are a good and more known example. I'm sure that if it could happen with mammals, that would be a very different story don't you think?

 
Arthropods do not have any kind of pain sensors. If you take out part of an insects leg, it does not limp but carries on walking with the leg it has left. There won't be signs of complaint.
Pain isn't around just to be a ###### but to improve ones survival chances. If a human got their leg ripped off and something drastic wasn't done immediatly they are going to die. If an insect gets its leg ripped off its body is designed to continue to function with little more than a hiccup. Loose a leg but save the body and survive. It makes no sense for them to experience large amounts of lingering pain since the pain would only hinder their escape and survival. That scenerio does not prove to me they have no buggy version of pain in other situations.Nor does the lack of having the same nerve endings as humans/other vertabrates. By that theory insects should be jello blobs with no form because they don't have bones like humans to support them. Somehow however, they manage just fine without bones and don't seem to have an issue holding their form at all.

 
Last edited by a moderator:
"Nor does the lack of having the same nerve endings as humans/other vertabrates. By that theory insects should be jello blobs with no form because they don't have bones like humans to support them. Somehow however, they manage just fine without bones and don't seem to have an issue holding their form at all. " I am not sure what you are exactly talking about? Insects have an exoskeleton to support them selves.

 
I am not sure what you are exactly talking about? Insects have an exoskeleton to support them selves.
Yes, I was using that as a comparison. Some users have sited that because they don't have nerve endings like vertebrates they must not feel pain. To me that is like saying well since bugs don't have bones like vertebrates they must not be able to hold their shape. Bones aren't the only way to give structure to a body so why would these nerve cells be the only possible way for an organism to experience pain? They very clearly have some tactile awareness so it doesn't seem impossible to me that one of the responses their body can send them would be the buggy version of pain.For the record, I don't tend to freeze my mantises either. While of course I will never know for sure, I don't get the impression that mine are in extreme amount of pain as they pass from old age. I don't know if they would find the fridge more merciful or scarier. I can't ask them so I can only do what I would like most if I was dying and that would be to have a loved one nearby to sit with me while I passed. Thus, that is what I do for my mantises.

 
Wow, OK. Thank you, I did not know that. Sorry, this was not good enough to post.
What I was getting at is that to prove your point you should post up a scientific peer reviewed writing on the subject.

 
Nerves are the only way I know an animal can feel pain or anything.

What I was getting at is that to prove your point you should post up a scientific peer reviewed writing on the subject.
Yes, I know.

 
Last edited by a moderator:
Some snippets from some peer reviewed articles:

1. While similarity of nervous systems to that of

humans is an important criterion used in judging

whether or not an animal might feel pain, is it really

adequate? Like human engineering, biology provides

numerous cases where the same problem has

been solved in different ways (e.g. flight in bats,

birds and insects). Specifying design criteria does

not specify the solution. For that reason, some

thought is needed when dealing with animals that

are massively different from humans.

What, then, can be said about a special-purpose

damage-avoidance system in invertebrates? Insects

have complex nervous systems and the fruit fly, for

instance, can associate odours with electric shock

and avoid such odours on subsequent occasions

(Dudai et al. 1976). Insects have no fibre system

equivalent to the pain fibres of mammals. Moreover,

they will continue normal behaviour after

severe injury or removal of body parts (Eisemann

et al. 1984). A locust will continue to feed while

it is itself being eaten by a mantis. These and

other examples are taken as evidence that insects

are different from vertebrates in not sensing

pain (Wigglesworth 1980), but it is as well to

remember that mammalian herbivores may also

graze seemingly normally after severe injury.

Twenty years ago it would have

been hard to find a scientist wondering whether

or not insects felt pain. Nowadays, a number do

(e.g. Wigglesworth 1980; Eisemann et al. 1984;

Kavaliers 1988).

(Patrick Bateson, Assessment of pain in animals, Animal Behaviour, Volume 42, Issue 5, November 1991, Pages 827-839, ISSN 0003-3472, 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80127-7.(http://www.sciencedi...003347205801277)

2. Avoiding dangerous situations, such as noxious heat or harmful tissue damage, is certainly most fundamental behavior of all animals. One can hardly imagine that any animal could survive in nature without innate reflective mechanisms to avoid such situations or even to defend itself against them. Such avoidance reactions usually start with the activation of somatosensory neurons covering the body surface that are responsive to repellent cues, such as heat or strong mechanical stimulation. In vertebrates, processing of these peripheral signals in the brain can ultimately cause the sensation of pain [1]. The fruitfly Drosophila melanogaster has frequently been used as a valuable model organism to dissect the neuronal basis of a variety of sensory processes, for example olfaction, gustation or sound detection [2] and [3], which often are surprisingly similar to those of vertebrates. But do insects feel pain? Certainly not in the sense higher mammals do, but it is certainly possible that they have sensory systems for detecting stimuli that can cause injury or damage, and that the basic mechanisms are evolutionarily conserved from fruitflies to mammals.

Biologists use the term ‘nociception’ [4] and [5] for the sensory detection of potentially harmful stimuli, to differentiate it from the psychologically loaded term ‘pain’ [1]. Likewise, a defensive behavior elicited by such stimuli can be called ‘nocifensive’ behavior. Such a nocifensive behavior can be observed in such a simple organism as a Drosophila larva. When fruitfly larvae are stimulated with a heated probe, such as the fine tip of a soldering iron, or are gently pinched, they perform a rolling behavior which is clearly different from their typical peristaltic locomotion behavior [6]. To investigate the mechanisms causing such a nocifensive behavior one can ask whether there are dedicated nociceptive neurons in fruit flies, similar to nociceptive neurons of vertebrates.

In Drosophila, two morphological types of somatosensory neuron have been described. Type I neurons terminate in a ciliated dendrite and are located in sensory bristles that sometimes can form complex organs, such as the Johnston's organ, the fly's ear. Type I neurons are diverse with respect to the stimuli they respond to, sound or gravity for example. In contrast, type II neurons are simpler, as they lack sensory cilia and extend their naked dendrites along epidermal cells of the body surface. In this respect, these neurons, which are also called multidendritic neurons, are similar to the nociceptive neurons of vertebrates [5].

A function of Drosophila multidendritic neurons as true nociceptors has been proposed after the exciting finding that they express a channel protein of the TRP family that is homologous to a temperature-sensitive channel (TRPA1/ANKTM1) expressed in vertebrate nociceptive neurons [6]. This channel protein opens at temperatures above 38°C, ultimately activating the sensory neuron. Interestingly, deletion of this channel in the mutant painless results in a defect in the nocifensive rolling behavior in Drosophila larvae [6], suggesting that multidendritic neurons might act as nociceptors. Other possible explanations for this remained, however; for example, it has been suggested that multidendritic neurons might be proprioreceptors, and if so, their impairment could simply prevent the fruitfly performing motor actions properly [7]. A further complication is that multidendritic neurons are diverse: four different classes can be distinguished by their different dendritic arborisation patterns, perhaps indicating they have different functions.

As reported recently in Current Biology, Hwang et al.[8] have addressed this question in a study which demonstrates once again how favourable Drosophila is for tackling such issues. Most importantly, transgenes can be expressed in Drosophila easily in a variety of neuronal populations of interest, in this case within different subpopulations of multidendritic neurons. By expressing tetanus toxin, a poison that abolishes chemical synaptic transmission, the authors were able to block the output selectively from morphologically distinct classes of multidendritic neurons. They found that blocking of only one single type — class IV neurons — strongly affects the putative nociceptive response evoked by heat or mechanical stimulation. These data demonstrate that class IV neurons, but not the other classes, are required for the initiation or performance of the nocifensive behavior. In addition, the authors have used a more recently developed technology to artificially activate neurons by illumination. Such photo-activation of neurons can be achieved by expressing the light-sensitive cation channel ‘Channelrhodopsin-2’ (ChR2) [9]. Illumination of the neurons expressing ChR2 causes their depolarization and thus activation.

The efficacy of this technology in Drosophila larvae has been demonstrated already in the context of olfactory learning and memory [10]. If multidendritic neurons are really nociceptive and responsible for triggering the nocifensive rolling behavior, light-induced activation of just these neurons should cause the illusion of a harmful stimulus and elicit the rolling response. Interestingly, only light-induced activation of class IV neurons induced a nocifensive rolling behavior, whereas activation of the other classes of neurons caused an accordion-like contraction of the larvae. This experiment is not only fascinating because of the modern genetic tricks that allowed Hwang et al.[8] to turn light into a ‘harmful’ stimulus. The experiment clearly demonstrates that activation of class IV neurons is sufficient to cause the nocifensive rolling behavior, whereas the other classes of multidendritic neurons might serve different functions in the context of coordinated locomotion.

One observation was puzzling, however: the larvae rolled more often towards the side from which the noxious stimulus came rather than away from it. To understand this seeming paradox, Hwang et al.[8]considered how such a behavior might have evolved. In nature, a serious threat for insect larvae are parasitoids, insects whose larvae feed from the body of other insects [11]. Drosophila melanogaster has such an enemy in the parasitoid wasp Leptopilina boulardi, whose females penetrate the larvae with their ovipositor and lay their eggs inside the larval body. Hwang et al.[8] showed that indeed Drosophila larvae can defend themselves against such wasp attacks by performing their rolling behavior. In a kung-fu-like fashion, the larva wraps the wasp's sting around its body, flips the attacking wasp through the air and onto its back, which gives the larva time to escape. This fascinating new study by Hwang et al.[8] vividly illustrates that animal behavior often can be understood only if the context of the animal's natural ecology is taken into account.

(André Fiala, Neuroethology: A Neuronal Self-Defense Mechanism in Fly Larvae, Current Biology, Volume 18, Issue 3, 12 February 2008, Pages R116-R117, ISSN 0960-9822, 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.054. (http://www.sciencedi...960982207023408)

3. Invertebrates are an expansive and diverse group of animals that have had little attention regarding

anesthesia and analgesia. Economic use, environmental awareness, laboratory research, and increasing

demand for invertebrates as pets has lead to a greater desire for knowledge for these animals in the

veterinary medical community. With the increasing number of animal welfare regulations, various

scientific studies have improved the overall knowledge of invertebrate medicine, but much more research

is required to fully understand anesthesia techniques in the different species treated by veterinarians.

Analgesia is a controversial and often neglected topic with invertebrates because of the common belief

that invertebrates do not feel pain. Recently, the idea that invertebrates do not feel pain has been

challenged with the discovery of nociceptive pathways similar to those in vertebrates. This article presents

a general overview of anesthetics and analgesics used in selective invertebrate taxa

(Gregory A. Lewbart, Conny Mosley, Clinical Anesthesia and Analgesia in Invertebrates, Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine, Volume 21, Issue 1, January 2012, Pages 59-70, ISSN 1557-5063, 10.1053/j.jepm.2011.11.007.(http://www.sciencedi...557506311002242)

 
Last edited by a moderator:
Nice read there Rick I may have to ask my zoology lecturer's about it next semester (they have stated that insects do not feel pain in previous years; however stating they can react to stimuli).

 
Dang! This will take me a while to read.
. I am not trying to say they feel pain as we do, but they most likely feel something that can be described as "pain". I had trouble finding more than a handful of peer reviewed articles.

 
well heard rumors that supposed plants can feel pain like insects but I dont know if its true or not

 
I agree that they feel some variation of 'pain', but so many people suggest other insects do not feel pain, I don't see how mantids are different...

 
Think about this: Is pain real if you don't remember it?

 

Latest posts

Top